A common conclusion about parasite life history is that parasites must have high fecundity in order to ensure their transmission from one host to another. However, there is another alternative – improving the probability of transmission for each offspring by investing resources in quality, rather than quantity. How can a parasite do this?
Combes: where does the stimulus/signal for transmission come from (e.g., downstream host or downstream host’s environment)? Which host is manipulated (upstream or downstream)?
Free-living infective stages can’t necessarily manipulate hosts, but parasite larvae can home in on the host’s environment (e.g. ticks crawling to the top of a grass stalk) or on the host itself (e.g. by detecting chemical signals given off by the host).
For non-free-living infective stages (particularly in the case of trophic transmission), parasites can try to manipulate either the upstream or the downstream host’s behavior. However, they only have access to their own physiology and to the physiology of the upstream host (which they are currently inhabiting); if they want to manipulate the downstream host, they have to present it with a signal. If a trematode makes a killifish behave strangely, thus increasing its chance of parasitism by a bird, who is being manipulated? The killifish or the bird?
Dicrocoelium dendriticum
Fungal entomopathogens
Toxoplasma gondii
Curtuteria australis
What structures are affected by parasites? What are the proximal mechanisms by which parasites change behavior?
Suppose a parasite species inhabits the CNS and changes host behavior. Is the parasite in the CNS to avoid host defenses, with changes in behavior being a coincidental result of tissue damage, or are they actively changing host behavior?
To answer these questions we can quantify changes in parasite and host fitness. In order to determine whether a change in host physiology and/or behavior deserves to be called “manipulation”, we have to classify its effects on host and parasite fitness. Here is one such possible classification:
Parasite fitness | Host fitness | Explanation |
+ (transmission) | - | Parasite manipulation |
+ (survivorship) | 0/- | Parasite site selection |
- (survivorship) | + | Host behavioral resistance |
- (transmission) | ? | Host inclusive-fitness reactions? |
0/- | - | Host pathology? |
0/+ | + | Host compensation (tolerance) |
Host behavioral changes in the presence of parasites are well documented. However, ecological (correlational) studies might get the direction of causality wrong: do hosts change their behavior when they are infected, or does behaviour increase their chances of infection? The ideal experiment would be to compare the transmission of parasites that do or don’t influence host behavior in a particular way. There are many obstacles to this kind of experiment:
Best-case scenario (?): study the transmission of two closely related parasites, either in the lab or in the wild. (Also: before-and-after studies, parasitized behaviors that are outside the usual repertoire.)
Other possibilities:
field epidemiological or observational studies (Moore, 1983); or the seal study reported in Combes (Des Clers & Wootten, 1990), or the comparisons done by Lafferty and Morris of predation on infected vs. uninfected fish) – these kinds of studies give you accurate information, but not detailed information, about what’s happening in natural systems
phylogenetically controlled studies (e.g. Moore and Gotelli 1996), which measure behaviors in a range of related species to test whether they represent adaptations or mere phylogenetic constraints.
A common assumption is that host behavior changes are driven by and for parasites, to increase parasite survivorship and transmission. However, their fitness consequences for the parasite or the host can be either positive, neutral, or negative: these behavioral changes can constitute adaptations by either the host or the parasite, or they can be “coincidental” side-effects of the host-parasite relationship.
In many cases behavioral changes of hosts are side-effects of parasite pathology, or host reactions, and do not necessarily enhance parasite fitness.
Not much exists, people have been too busy looking at the amazingly cool detailed mechanisms of manipulation of host behavior by parasites.
Some of the difficulties with doing comparative analysis are (1) the lack of good phylogenies of parasites (which has come up before) and (2) the lack of standardized measures of behavioral change; in the case of life history or virulence, at least there are more standard benchmarks (longevity, size, fecundity, etc.) for people to measure.
Moore & Gotelli (1996)
Acanthocephalans are transmitted from cockroaches to other hosts by predation, which makes them a good candidate for (parasite-) adaptive parasite-induced behavioral change driven by predation success. Moore and Gotelli took a morphological study of cockroaches, constructed a cockroach phylogeny, and mapped their own studies of a variety of different behavioral responses (substrate choice, etc.) onto the phylogeny. They found that at the family level there was little phylogenetic inertia, although there was some at the subfamily level; overall conclusion, behavioral responses to parasitism evolve fairly rapidly relative to the time scale of speciation and higher-taxon divergence.
The value of host manipulation, and the optimal/adaptive level of manipulation, depends on costs and benefits to the parasite (of course).
The costs of host manipulation may lead to kin/group selection of parasites: e.g. costs of producing host hormones. Kin/group selection must be especially strong in parasites of the central nervous system, because typically the individual parasites that are in the CNS doing the manipulation (latching on to neurons or whatever) are not successfully transmitted to the next host in the cycle: they provide an opportunity for their neighbors, but don’t themselves get to hitch a ride.
Dicrocoelium dendriticum is one example of a CNS parasite where one manipulator benefits all the other parasites in the host. Interspecies free riding occurs too: e.g. Microphallus and Macrotremata, where there are potential free riders both within and between species.
Brown (1999) gives a game-theoretic treatment of whether it’s worth a parasite’s effort to invest in host manipulation, on the basis of how many individuals it shares the host with and how related they are. The main prediction is that there can be a group size/relatedness threshold below which manipulation is not worth it for the parasite. Brown suggests that we look for variation in manipulation as a function of parasite number. Q: how might hormonal and direct control of behavior differ in the individual and group pressures they put on parasites?
Fisher’s principle: most organisms do best with even sex ratios (on average), unless they do a lot of inbreeding
There are variants of parasitic castration: male-killing (the “pharaoh strategy”), or feminization: turning males into females (genetically or phenotypically). The host will try to compensate (by producing more males) if it can, to reach its optimal sex ratio; the parasite will fight back.
In Gammarus infected with Octosporea effeminans (a microsporidian), 90% of offspring of parasitized mothers become females (as opposed to 50–80% in unparasitized broods).
Wolbachia is an incredibly common intracellular, vertically transmitted bacterium of arthropods that has a huge range of sex ratio distortion tricks.
Charlat et al. (2003)
[In the pill woodlouse Armadillidium vulgare] The spread of the feminizing Wolbachia has caused the loss of the female-determining W chromosome from infected populations, and all individuals in these populations are ZZ. The female-determining factor has switched from being the W chromosome to the feminizing Wolbachia: individuals are female if Wolbachia is present and active, and male if it is either absent or inactive.
There is a problem at the lineage level with this strategy: unless females can reproduce parthenogenetically, you may be dooming your host population if you’re too effective at feminizing it. For plants there is another way out: encourage clonal growth or selfing. This both increases transmission efficiency and reduces the potential for the evolution of parasite resistance; in the end, though, it might also select for reduced virulence of the parasite because it ensures vertical transmission (Clay & Kover, 1996).
Last updated: 2023-11-27 12:05:12.261923